How gender roles are fixed in society. No double standards

Before the dawn of the 21st century, it seemed that only technologies will evolve in the world of the future, but with its transformation into the world of the present, it turned out that it is still far from ideal. Even after seeing the sixth iPhone, we still continue to dress boys in blue, and girls in pink, and when they grow up, we expect “male” and “female” actions from them. However, in society new round a slow but sure process of revising the established standards and connections began - it turned out that following it was no less interesting than following the adventures of the Higgs boson. We talk a lot about the perception of physicality, with ourselves, as well as how important it is for our common comfort and love for the diversity and uniqueness of people in a multicultural global reality. However, this process is impossible without an understanding of how the existing models of relations have developed, how ideas about the “correct” or “traditional” have been entrenched in our minds, and why changes are inevitable. We start a big conversation about gender roles - the social perception of gender - and what happens in modern world with the concepts of "man" and "woman".

Text: Alice Taezhnaya
Photo: Vera Mishurina

Stay in my skin
How Gender Roles Work

To understand how strongly our behavior is dictated by gender roles, it is enough to analyze a day in life modern man. Unless, of course, you live in seclusion, then those around you, guided by the understandable and learned experience of millennia-old patriarchy, most likely expect you to be included in the generally accepted system of values ​​and concepts. A determined son and an attentive daughter, a disciplined husband and a calm wife, an authoritative father and an affectionate mother, an enterprising subordinate and an understanding boss - we unconsciously fit into this coordinate system so as not to be strangers among our own.

The dramaturgy of comedy and tragedy is based on gender roles. Remember episode"Friends" about the male nanny: everyone becomes more comfortable when the nanny becomes a girl, and not the sentimental and often crying guy Sandy with a perfect education and amazing characteristics. Or remember what happens to Betty Draper in Mad Men when a single mother who has divorced her husband arrives in a peaceful housewife village, works hard and raises children herself.

We call unbalanced men “hysterics” behind our backs, and decisive girls with principles - “heifers with eggs”, we compete in a sense of humor using gender stereotypes, and deafeningly laugh at the same jokes Barney Stinson or Michael Scott. In our speech, we constantly choose emotionally colored and far from gender-neutral descriptions of ourselves, the people and phenomena around us, and it is these descriptions that demonstrate and reinforce the perception of one or another gender.

Who has benefited from the shift in gender roles

Can gender role
be a free choice

At the end of the 19th century, Great Britain - the main and strongest empire - and behind it the whole of Europe canonize the role of a woman in Coventry Patmore's poem "The Angel in the House", which he dedicated to his virtuous wife, and John Everett Millais will draw her idealized portrait. At about the same time and in this city, Jack the Ripper will brutally kill a huge number of London prostitutes, who had been humiliated and raped by the police for a decade before that for compulsory venereal disease tests, and Oscar Wilde will undermine his health in prison, sitting on charges of sodomy. Reactionary laws and private histories show that even now female images in culture they mask, but do not change the state of affairs. Two world wars and three waves of feminism were not enough for the system to stop reproducing itself: gender stereotypes in 2014 prevent not only taking a wife’s surname after marriage, but also calculating one’s strength in a career and earnings when meeting with the “glass ceiling”.


Are gender stereotypes alive?

If the power and influence of gender stereotypes seems to have waned over time, experiment. Open Dahl's dictionary of proverbs, collected by the middle of the 19th century, and then read the reader's comments on popular materials on your favorite site. “Husband is at least as big as a fist, but I don’t sit behind my husband’s head as an orphan.” "Do not beat your wife - and do not be sweet." "Baba is dear - from the stove to the doorstep." "The hair is long, but the mind is short." “A dog is smarter than a woman: it doesn’t bark at its owner.” "A chicken is not a bird, and a woman is not a person." "Wherever the devil dares, he will send a woman there." We no longer use most of them, but their meaning is firmly planted in the collective unconscious and crawls out into the light at every opportunity.

Dialogues between men and women on sensitive issues on forums or in comments are most often built on the basis of repeatedly lost gender roles. These scenarios were exposed by John Money and Robert Stoller, John Gray tried to popularize and explain them in "Men are from Mars, women are from Venus", the theme of gender is constantly heard in contemporary art and news, but more often than not, news even on problematic sites like Jezebel or PolicyMic is designed to spread viral content, reproduce ready-made meanings, and rarely open their eyes to the other side of the problem.

Why gender is last
and the most steadfast stronghold of tradition

The problem of gender is inscribed in the spectrum of modern existential and economic problems in which our unstable, over-consuming and competitive society is immersed. Ethnically intermarriage and migration are changing demographic composition seemingly stable communities: whether Hong Kong can be called European and Marseille Asian, and whether it is generally correct to use the terms Europe and Asia in the 21st century remains a question. Alternative sources income and the modern economy with contract work and bitcoin are reshaping the employment relationship. But books on prosperity and life hacks continue to be bestsellers, only now Dale Carnegie's advice is being replaced by instructive biographies of tech moguls.

At the same time, as a strategy for the global community, both the ideal of a communist future and the American dream. One has discredited itself with inefficient regimes with double standards, the other creates a destructive competition and objectively cannot stop another economic crisis. And if with political ideologies or professional choice people can still risk by presenting these systems outside, then gender - one of the most basic, intimate and constant constants - seems to be the last link this man and this woman with the idea of ​​man in general.

Prejudice against career women exists regardless of the gender of the subject assessing them.

“It so happened historically” is one of the easiest ways to justify and connect a person here and now with a million long-dead anonymous people, to whom a thread of repeatedly rewritten history from textbooks, confused traces of genealogy and global cultural monuments that cannot be ignored, whether it is pyramids, bible or hollywood.

The transgender performance experiment tells a lot about the verdicts that most of us have in store for both sexes and their acceptable behavior. A biological woman, having performed a sex change operation, finds herself in a comfortable and practically invulnerable position. But the “man” who “becomes” a woman immediately raises doubts about professionalism and receives several humiliating comments about his work. Another study shows that prejudice against career women exists regardless of the gender of the subject assessing them. Comments addressed to men contain a lot of constructive criticism and positive remarks about the need to work on oneself, comments to women always have an emotional and harsh evaluative coloring with a transition to the individual.

Gender scholar Londa Schiebinger speaks of the ubiquitous tendency of young children to make choices based on the reaction of their environment:
in children, according to her impressions, different qualities and inclinations are still encouraged by parents. Part of her books explain the division into masculine and women's professions and answer, among other things, the question “why there were no great women scientists” or one of the most frequently asked questions “why there are no great women artists”, which Linda Nokhlin once answered so well. This, however, does not negate the fact that in some societies the issue of gender roles is obviously not so acute (for example, Scandinavia) and the existence of women in power, and men in the family, as well as a wide range of LGBTI relations, does not need additional argument there. .

Can the modern family save us from the trap of gender roles

As Time frightens and reassures us at the same time, there is no such thing as a typical family anymore. Indeed, if the number of divorced parents with joint custody, separated spouses and same-sex couples raising children reaches significant percentages, it is strange and illogical to program in gender roles that cannot be realized in real life. Most likely, a man in a sling and a woman working on maternity leave are not the main, and certainly not the last, result of changing social roles. But considering how late different forms lives in the family and society get their names (some appeared in the language a couple of decades ago), one can only affirm that the most unhurried mutations occur with gender roles. A complete rejection of them is at the same distance as the construction of a new economic system or hyperintensive global cataclysm: now no expert will take the responsibility to predict the exact expiration date current situation of things.

In addition, having abandoned the usual gender roles, we will have to restructure our attitude towards daily habits, friends and relatives, swap very funny sexist jokes for something just as good, come up with a new cinema without the usual genres, characters and plots, voluntarily abandon most gender-based products and boycott jobs that pay us unevenly. We will have to put aside going to psychoanalysts that revere Freud's theories and accept the possibility that hormone therapy and body experimentation will become common practice after years of public resistance. The utopian consciousness is building up such a scenario, unlike today, in which gender can be changed almost as often as a haircut, professions as hobbies, partners as books at the head, and these books themselves at the head will have to write about something else and on another language to be of interest to us in our as yet unimagined new roles.

gender roles

One of the types of social roles, a set of expected patterns of behavior (or norms) for men and women. Role in social psychology is defined as a set of norms that determine how people should behave in a given social position. Shakespeare can rightfully be considered the first representative of the role theory, who wrote:

The whole world is a theater

In it, women, men - all actors.

They have their own exits, departures;

And each one plays a role.

Currently, there is no unified theory of social roles as such. Gender roles, their characteristics, origin and development are considered within the framework of various sociological, psychological and biosocial theories. But the available research allows us to conclude that their formation and development in humans is influenced by society and culture, the ideas about the content and specifics of gender roles enshrined in them. And during historical development society, the content of gender roles is subject to change. Margaret Mead (M. Mead) in her book "Sex and Temperament" dealt a blow to the belief that men and women are naturally created to fulfill certain roles. Her observations of tribal life in New Guinea convincingly refute this. The women and men she observed played completely different roles, sometimes directly opposite to the stereotypes accepted for each sex. One of the ideas promoted by the women's movement in the 1970s was that traditional gender roles personal development and realization of the existing potential. It served as an impetus for the concept of Sandra Bem (S. Bem), which is based on the concept of androgyny, according to which any person, regardless of his biological sex, can combine traditionally male and traditionally female qualities (such people are called androgynes). And this allows people to less rigidly adhere to gender-role norms and freely move from traditionally female occupations to traditionally male ones and vice versa. Developing this idea, Pleck (Pleck) in his works began to talk about the splitting, or fragmentation of gender roles. Not single role men or women. Each person performs a number of different roles, such as wives, mothers, students, daughters, girlfriends, etc. Sometimes these roles do not overlap, leading to role conflict. The conflict between the role of a business woman and the role of a mother is well known to everyone. There is now evidence that playing many roles contributes to psychological well-being person.

The diversity of gender roles across cultures and eras supports the hypothesis that our gender roles are culturally shaped. According to Hofstede's theory, differences in gender roles depend on the degree of gender differentiation in cultures or the degree of masculinity or femininity of a particular culture. Based on cross-cultural studies, Hofstede showed that people of masculine cultures have a higher achievement motivation, see the meaning of life in work and are able to work hard and hard. A number of cross-cultural studies have also found that feminine cultures with low power distance (Denmark, Finland, Norway, Sweden) have personality-oriented families that promote the assimilation of equality in gender roles. While cultures with a high power distance and pronounced masculinity (Greece, Japan, Mexico) have families focused on rigid gender role positions. Such families ultimately contribute to a rigid differentiation in gender roles.

Gender roles depend not only on culture, but also on historical era. I. S. Kon noted that traditional system differentiation of sex roles and related stereotypes of femininity-masculinity differed in the following characteristic features: female and male activities and personal qualities differed very sharply and seemed polar; these differences were sanctified by religion or references to nature and seemed indestructible; female and male functions were not just complementary, but also hierarchical, the woman was given a dependent, subordinate role. Now almost all cultures are undergoing radical changes in relation to gender roles, in particular, in post-Soviet space, but not as fast as we would like.

gender roles

Literature:

Kon IS Psychology of gender differences // Questions of psychology. 1981. N 2. S. 53.

Lebedeva NM Introduction to ethical and cross-cultural psychology. M.: Key, 1999. S. 141-142.

Bem S. The measurement of psychological androgyny // Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1974. 42. R. 165-172.

Hofstede G. Culture's consequences: international differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, 1984.

Mead M. Sex and temperament in three primitive societies. New York: Morrow, 1935.

Pleck J. The theory of male sex role identity: its rise and fall from 1936 to the present // The making of masculinities: the new men's studies. Boston: Allen & Unwin, 1987. P. 221-38.

© E. F. Ivanova


Thesaurus of gender studies terminology. - M.: East-West: Women's Innovation Projects. A. A. Denisova. 2003 .

See what "Gender roles" are in other dictionaries:

    gender roles- ... Wikipedia

    Gender roles (gender roles)- - attitudes, as well as activities that society associates with one sex or another ... Social Work Dictionary

    Gender differences- a set of specific psychological and physiological characteristics of men and women. Gender differences are based on the sexual dimorphism of males and females. Exist academic subject"gender psychology", which studies both qualitatively and ... Wikipedia

    GENDER ISSUES- (eng. gender gender, gender), social and psychological problems associated with the role of male and female persons in society, since differences in behavior patterns of men and women can become causes of intrapersonal, interpersonal and intergroup ... encyclopedic Dictionary

    Gender Issues- (eng. gender gender, gender) social and psychological problems associated with the role of male and female persons in society, since differences in behavior patterns of men and women can become causes of intrapersonal, interpersonal and intergroup ... ... Political science. Vocabulary.

    GENDER DIFFERENCES- (eng. gender genus, sex), differences between people due to their gender. So, it is believed that men have more developed spatial and mathematical abilities, they are more aggressive and dominant, more significant for them ... ... encyclopedic Dictionary

    Gender differences- Contents 1 Gender differences 2 Gender identity 3 ... Wikipedia

    A stereotype is a judgment, in a sharply simplifying and generalizing form, with emotional coloring, attributing certain properties to a certain class of persons or, conversely, denying them these properties. Stereotypes are seen as special forms… …

    - (personal computers) are observed during the interaction of a person and a computer in all age groups. Stereotypes of public consciousness and the media, the bias of education and software products being produced to some extent determine that ... Gender Studies Terms

    GENDER TECHNOLOGIES- methods, mechanisms, channels for the formation of the institution of sex and the consolidation of the corresponding gender identifications. The logic of the modern definition of social gender (see Gender) points to the inseparable connection between the concepts of gender, discourse, and power. G. t.… … Modern Philosophical Dictionary

Books

  • Why Men Lie and Women Roar by Allan Pease. In a world where gender roles are so clearly blurred and transformed, the authors have brilliantly managed to articulate the differences in the perception of reality between men and women, and explain the motives ...

Gender roles are roles determined by the differentiation of people in society on the basis of gender. gender role- differentiation of activities, statuses, rights and obligations of individuals depending on their gender; refers to the type of social roles, is normative, expresses certain social expectations (expectations), manifests itself in behavior. At the cultural level, they exist in the context of a certain system of gender symbolism and stereotypes of masculinity and femininity. Gender "roles are always associated with a certain normative system that a person learns and refracts in his mind and behavior" .

Thus, gender roles can be seen as external manifestations of patterns of behavior and attitudes that allow other people to judge whether an individual is male or female. In other words, it is the social manifestation of an individual's gender identity.

Gender roles refer to a type of prescribed roles. The status of a future man or a future woman is acquired by a child at birth, and then, in the process of gender socialization, the child learns to play one or another gender role. Gender stereotypes that exist in society have big influence on the process of socialization of children, largely determining its direction. Under gender stereotypes standardized ideas about behavior patterns and character traits that correspond to the concepts of "male" and "female" are understood.

The gender stereotype regarding the consolidation of family and professional roles in accordance with gender is one of the most common stereotypes that prescribe standard models of role behavior for men and women. In accordance with this stereotype, for women, family roles (mother, housewife) are considered the main social roles, for men, professional roles (worker, worker, earner, breadwinner). Men are usually evaluated by professional success, women - by the presence of a family and children. folk wisdom states that a "normal" woman wants to get married and have children and that all other interests they may have are secondary to these family roles. To fulfill the traditional role of a housewife, a woman must have sensitivity, compassion and caring. While men are required to be achievement-oriented, women are required to be people-oriented and seek close interpersonal relationships.

One of the bases for the formation of traditional gender roles is the division of labor based on gender. The main criterion in this division is the biological ability of women to bear children. AT modern societies long gone social necessity division of labor based on the reproductive ability of women, which existed in archaic societies. Most women work in the manufacturing sector outside the home, and men have long ceased to be only “warriors and hunters” who protect and feed their families. And, nevertheless, stereotypes about traditional gender roles are very stable: women are required to concentrate on the private (home) sphere of activity, and men - in the professional, public sphere.

An important role in the approval of the gender stereotype about the consolidation of social roles in accordance with gender was played by the concept of "natural" complementarity of the sexes by Talcott Parsons and Robert Bales, who considered the differentiation of male and female roles in structural and functional terms. According to their point of view, spouses should play two different roles in the modern family. The instrumental role is to maintain communication between the family and outside world is a professional activity that brings material income and social status; the expressive role involves primarily caring for children and regulating relationships within the family. How is the distribution of responsibilities between spouses based on these two roles? Parsons and Bales believe that a wife's ability to bear children and care for children unequivocally determine her expressive role, and a husband who cannot perform these biological functions becomes an instrumental role performer.

This theory contributed to the integration into a single scheme of socio-anthropological and psychological data. However, feminist criticism has shown that the instrumental and expressive dichotomy, for all its empirical and worldly persuasiveness, is based not so much on natural gender differences as on social norms, adherence to which hampers the individual self-development and self-expression of women and men.

Traditional gender roles hinder the development of the individual and the realization of the existing potential. This idea was the impetus for the development of Sandra Bam androgyny concepts, according to which a person, regardless of his biological sex, can have both masculine and femininity features, combining both traditionally feminine and traditionally masculine qualities. This allows you to highlight the masculine, feminine, androgynous models of gender roles. This idea was developed further, and J. Plec in his works began to talk about the splitting, or fragmentation, of gender roles. There is no single male or female role. Each person performs a number of different roles (wife, mother, business woman, etc.), often these roles may not be combined, which leads to an intrapersonal role conflict.

Gender roles can be studied at three different levels. At the macrosocial level, we are talking about the differentiation of social functions by gender and the corresponding cultural norms. To describe the “female role” at this level means to reveal the specifics of the social position of a woman (typical activities, social status, mass ideas about a woman) by correlating it with the position of a man within a given society, system.

At the level of interpersonal relations, the gender role is derived not only from general social norms and conditions, but also from the particular system of joint activity being studied. The role of a mother or wife always depends on how the responsibilities are specifically distributed in a given family, how the roles of father, husband, children, etc. are defined in it.

At the intra-individual level, the internalized gender role is a derivative of the characteristics of a particular personality: an individual builds his behavior as a husband or father, taking into account his ideas about what, in his opinion, a man should be in general, based on all his conscious and unconscious attitudes and life experience.

Encyclopedic YouTube

    1 / 4

    Men in Search: Relationship Degradation, Feminism, Gender Roles

    New order. How are gender roles changing?

    Steven Universe and Gender Roles | Blitz and Chips

    Women of the Great Patriotic War

    Subtitles

Gender role and gender identity

Gender role should be distinguished from gender identity: the first concept describes social expectations external to a person in connection with his gender, the second - a person’s internal self-perception as a representative of a particular gender. A person's gender identity and gender role may not match - particularly in transgender and intersex people. Aligning gender roles with gender identity is part of the transgender transition.

Gender roles in different cultures

Modern societies are dominated by a binary gender system - a way of social organization in which people are divided into two opposite groups - men and women. The binary gender system implies a strict correspondence between the sex assigned at birth and gender role, as well as other parameters (in particular, gender identity and sexual orientation). As anthropological studies show, the establishment of such a correspondence is not universal: in many cultures, biological, in particular anatomical sex, does not play a role. key role in defining gender role or gender identity. Not universal and the allocation of only two genders. For example, many native North American cultures have three or four genders and corresponding gender roles. In West African Yoruba culture, gender is traditionally not a significant social category, and social roles are determined primarily by age and kinship.

Even within close cultures or within the same culture, gender roles can differ markedly. For example, in European secular culture In the 18th and 19th centuries, women were expected to be weak and frail, and in most peasant cultures, women were considered to be naturally strong and resilient. In Western (North American and Western European) middle-class cultures since the 1950s, the female gender role has been that of the housewife, and participation in productive work for women has been excluded. Yet at the same time and in the same societies, working outside the home was an expected and self-evident element of the gender role for working-class women. Women's gender role in socialist societies also involved a combination of work outside the home, housework, and family care.

Gender Development Explanations

There are two main points of view in the debate about the origin of gender roles and differences: biological determinists suggest that gender differences are determined by biological, natural factors, and supporters of social constructivism - that they are formed by society in the process of socialization. Various theories of gender development have been put forward in science. Biologically based theories explaining differences in gender roles by evolution have not found convincing empirical evidence. Empirical research has also refuted psychoanalytic theories that explained gender development through the child's relationship with parents. The strongest empirical evidence exists for cognitive and socio-cognitive theories that explain gender development as a complex interplay of biological, cognitive, and social factors.

Viewpoints on the origin of gender roles

Ordinary consciousness often represents the existing in a given society in a specific historical period gender roles as natural and natural. There is also a great deal of research that seeks to reveal the biological basis of gender roles - in particular, to establish the biological origin of gender differences between men and women, as well as to find the biological causes of gender non-conformity. But the historical and anthropological knowledge accumulated to date does not support this point of view, since the diversity of ideas about gender and gender roles in the cultures of the world and throughout history is too great. At the same time, modern social sciences have collected a lot of data on how gender roles are formed under the influence of various social processes.

Biological determinism

The point of view that social phenomena determined biological factors, is called biological determinism. A related concept is naturalization social practices - describes the process of interpreting social practices as facts of nature. Biological determinism in relation to gender roles is expressed, for example, in the widespread assertions that motherhood is a woman's natural destiny, or that men are not naturally emotional.

With late XIX century, scientists from different scientific fields have done a lot of research on gender differences between men and women. Up until the 1970s, the main goal of these studies was to confirm biological nature gender differences and substantiate the content of existing gender roles. However, the results of most studies show that there are much more similarities between men and women than differences. In a widely cited review study, psychologists Eleanor Maccoby and Carol Jacklin cite four dimensions in which differences between men and women have been found: spatial awareness, mathematical ability, language skills, and aggressiveness. But even these discovered differences are small and strongly depend on the methodology and conditions of the study.

Since the 1970s, scholars have also become interested in the causes of gender non-conformity, i.e. violations of gender roles. In particular, research has been carried out to determine biological reasons transsexuality. There are currently theories linking transsexuality to genetics, brain structure, brain activity, and androgen exposure during fetal development. At the same time, the results of these studies are also controversial - for example, the revealed features of the structure of the brain of transsexual people are not unique (similar differences are observed in homosexual people compared to heterosexual people), and there is evidence that the structure of the brain can change under the influence of life experience.

social constructivism

The point of view according to which gender roles are formed, or constructed, by society belongs to the theory of social constructivism. basis for learning social nature and the processes of constructing gender roles were laid, in particular, by the theoretical works of Simone de Beauvoir and Michel Foucault. Studies of the social construction of gender roles show how, in the process of socialization and interaction between people, those gender differences and expectations are formed that are perceived as natural and natural in ordinary consciousness.

According to latest research found differences between men and women are largely due to social factors. For example, research reveals several reasons why women are less successful in mathematics than men: firstly, they lack confidence in their abilities, and secondly, they consider math classes inappropriate for their gender role and refuse them even when show excellent abilities in this area, thirdly, parents and teachers encourage girls to do mathematics much less than boys. Thus, as some researchers note, gender stereotypes work like self-fulfilling prophecies: in the course of socialization, people are given information about gender roles that forms their expectations of themselves, and as a result they show gender-conforming behavior.

Biological theories

Biologically based explanations of gender development and differences are widespread. One of the most influential of these theories, evolutionary psychology, explains gender differentiation by heredity. hereditary origin gender roles are analyzed through preferences in the choice of sexual partners, reproductive strategies, the contribution of parents to the care of offspring, and the aggressiveness of men. From the point of view of this theory, modern gender roles are due to the successful adaptation of the ancestors of modern man to differences in reproductive tasks men and women.

Empirical evidence refutes basic assumptions biological theories gender development. Many researchers also criticize the methodology of biologically oriented research. Nevertheless, biological theories continue to enjoy great popularity, including among the general public. According to some authors, this is due to the fact that in many societies, ordinary consciousness ascribes to biology the status of absolute truth. In addition, the provisions of biological theories correspond to gender stereotypes.

Reproductive strategies

According to evolutionary psychology, in the process of evolution, different reproductive strategies were fixed at the genetic level in men and women, dictated by the need to ensure the survival of man as a biological species. The reproductive strategy of men is aimed at maximizing the spread of their genes, so men prefer to have many sexual partners and not spend time caring for offspring. The reproductive strategy of women is to have few sexual partners who will be able to provide themselves and their offspring with the necessary resources for survival.

Many researchers question the very concept of reproductive strategy. From the point of view of the general theory of evolution, natural selection is determined by immediate practical benefits, and not by future goals. The claim that ancient men sought to become fathers as much as possible more children, and ancient women to find reliable breadwinners, suggests that they had a conscious or unconscious purpose, which, according to some authors, contradicts the Darwinian functional explanation.

Other authors point out that the evolutionary psychology hypothesis is not supported by empirical evidence. In particular, the assumption that ancient women did not have enough food during pregnancy and lactation, looks quite convincing, but with the same success, based on this, it can be assumed that in connection with this, women developed increased abilities for orientation in space and memory , which would allow them to find and remember the location of food sources. Justification of any hypothesis about specific adaptive mechanisms requires Additional Information. Such information could be data from molecular studies of fossilized human remains or data from archeology, but evolutionary psychology does not offer such data. Some authors point out that the concept of reproductive strategies is an attempt to "hindsight" explain modern gender stereotypes.

Anthropological evidence also speaks against the hypothesis of reproductive strategies. They show, in particular, that reproductive behavior is influenced by cultural beliefs about the human body and reproduction. In cultures where multi-partner fertilization is considered necessary for reproduction, women have sexual contact with different partners, and these partners are not jealous of each other.

Choice of sexual partners

Evolutionary psychology states that men tend to choose young and physically attractive mates because such mates are more likely to bear healthy offspring, and women tend to choose financially wealthy men who can feed them. In confirmation of these data, the results of surveys are given, in which men and women named the most attractive characteristics of potential partners for themselves. However, numerous studies show that what people say is significantly different from how they actually behave: in fact, physical attractiveness affects the choice of partners in men and women in the same way. On the other hand, indicators of physical attractiveness are extremely diverse in terms of different cultures world, and most of these characteristics are not related to fertility. Some authors also point out that evolutionary psychology only explains heterosexual behavior and suggest that evolutionary psychologists avoid looking at data from studies of non-heterosexual people because their behavior and gender roles do not match gender stereotypes and thus undermine evolutionary explanations.

Aggressive behavior

Psychoanalytic theories

Although psychoanalytic theory has had a major impact on the development of developmental psychology, empirical evidence does not support it. Research has not found a strong relationship between same-sex parent identification and gender role learning. Children's role models are much more likely to be caring or socially powerful adults than to be threatening adults with whom the child has a competitive relationship.

The lack of empirical support for classical psychoanalytic theory has led to the emergence of various updated versions of it. In the field of gender development, one of the most influential latest versions This is Nancy Chodorow's theory. According to this theory, gender identity is formed during infancy and not during the phallic phase, as Freud claimed. Both boys and girls initially identify with their mother, but because daughters are the same sex as their mother, identification between daughters and mothers is stronger than between sons and mothers. In the course of further development, girls retain identification with their mother and psychologically merge with her. As a result of the representation of the girl and the woman about themselves, it is characterized strong feeling interdependence, which translates into a desire for interpersonal relationships and encourages a woman, in turn, to become a mother. The development of the boy is determined by the desire to separate from the mother and further define himself through difference from women, which leads to the belittling of femininity.

But the empirical evidence does not support Chodorow's theory either. Research does not find a stronger bond between mothers and daughters than between mothers and sons. There is also no evidence that women's needs for interpersonal relationships satisfied only through motherhood. On the contrary, studies show that women whose only social role- this is the role of mother and wife, more prone to the appearance psychological problems than childless married or unmarried women and working mothers.

Cognitive and social theories

Cognitive and social theories of gender development include theories of cognitive development, gender schemas, social learning, and social cognitive theory. Although on early stages these theories differed significantly from each other, and their supporters had heated discussions among themselves, modern versions of these theories have much in common. In general, cognitive and social theories consider gender development as a complex process of interaction of biological, social and cognitive factors. All of them pay great attention social source m of gender development and the active role that a person plays in their own gender development.

Social sources of gender development

The social sources of gender development include, in particular, the influence of parents, other significant adults and peers, as well as information pressure from the media, cinema, literature, etc.

Influence of parents

Differences in the upbringing of boys and girls are described by the concept of "differential socialization". Differential socialization is not necessarily expressed in the form of direct instructions or prohibitions. As studies show, differential socialization begins even before the birth of a child, as soon as its sex is determined using ultrasound. Mothers who learn the sex of their unborn child in this way describe boys as "active" and "mobile", and girls as "calm". From birth, babies tend to be surrounded by gender-specific toys, diapers, and other items; male infants are described as "big", "strong" and "independent", while girls are referred to as "gentle", "delicate" and "beautiful", even if there are no objective differences in the appearance or behavior of the infants. Thus, the ideas and expectations of children related to gender are formed by adults on the basis of gender stereotypes long before the child can begin to show this or that behavior.

Differential socialization continues into the later life of the child. For example, numerous studies show that parents are more stimulating and more responsive to motor activity in infant boys than in girls. Another illustrative experiment concerns the influence of adult gender stereotypes on the choice of toys for children. The experiment was initially carried out with the participation of a three-month-old child, and later again with the participation of several children aged from three to 11 months. Three groups of adults were asked to play with the child, while the first group was told that the child was a girl, the second group that it was a boy, and the third group was not informed about the gender of the child. The adults had three toys at their disposal: a doll, a ball, and a gender-neutral tooth ring. Most of the adults who considered the child to be a boy offered him a ball, and the majority of those who considered the child to be a girl - a doll, without trying to find out which of the toys interests the child more.

Peer Influence

As it expands social peace peer groups become another important source of gender development, as well as social learning in general. In interaction with peers, children, starting at the age of three or four, encourage each other for gender-typical behavior, as well as for playing in gender-homogeneous groups, and punish for behavior that is considered inappropriate for their gender.

Information pressure

Finally, the media play a significant role in gender development, especially television, as well as literature, cinema and video games. In these sources from which children receive information about gender roles, men and women are often portrayed in an exaggerated stereotype: men are portrayed as active and adventurous, while women are portrayed as dependent, unambitious and emotional. Image professional life men and women often do not match real situation affairs: men are portrayed as representatives of various professions, leaders and bosses, and women - either as housewives or as working in low-status positions. Such a portrayal does not correspond to the actual statistics of the professional employment of men, nor to the wide involvement of women in professional activity. Studies show that gender stereotypes in the media and culture have a big impact on children: those who watch a lot of TV form more stereotyped ideas about gender roles. On the other hand, the non-stereotypical depiction of representatives of different genders expands the range of desires and aspirations in children, as well as the options for roles that they consider acceptable for their gender. The recurring image of equal participation of representatives of different genders in certain activities contributes to the steady softening of gender stereotypes in young children.

Active human role in gender development

Social sources of gender development often provide conflicting information about gender roles and impose conflicting expectations on the child. This requires the child, from a very early age, to actively seek and build their own own rules and ideas about gender as a new and significant social category for him. Activity in the formation of ideas about gender is manifested, in particular, in selective attention and memory, as well as in the formation of preferences - for example, gender-typical or atypical toys, games with peers of one's own or another gender.

The Importance of Gender Roles

Gender roles in Russian society

see also

Notes

  1. Nanda, Serena. Gender Diversity: Crosscultural Variations . - Waveland Pr Inc, 1999. - ISBN 978-1577660743.
  2. Roscoe, Will. Changing Ones: Third and Fourth Genders in Native North America . - Palgrave Macmillan, 2000. - ISBN 978-0312224790.
  3. Oyewumi, Oyeronke. Conceptualizing gender: the eurocentric foundations of feminist concepts and the challenge of African epistemologies // Jenda: a Journal of Culture and African Woman Studies. - 2002. - Vol. 2.
  4. Connell R. Gender and Power: Society, Identity and Gender Policy. - M.: New Literary Review, 2015. - ISBN 978-5-4448-0248-9.
  5. hooks, bell. Rethinking the Nature of Work // Feminist Theory: From Margin to Center. - Pluto Press, 2000. - ISBN 9780745316635.
  6. Zdravomyslova E., Temkina A. (ed.). Russian gender order: a sociological approach. - St. Petersburg: Publishing House European University in St. Petersburg, 2007. - ISBN 978-5-94380-060-3.
  7. Bussey, K., Bandura, A. Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation // Psychological review. - 1999. - T. 106, No. 4. - S. 676-713.
  8. Fausto-Sterling A. Beyond difference: A biologist "s perspective // ​​Journal of Social Issues. - 1997. - V. 53, No. 2. - S. 233–258.
  9. Martin, C.L., et al. Cognitive Theories of Early Gender Development // Psychological Bulletin. - 2002. - T. 128, No. 6. - S. 903-933.
  10. Burn Sean. Gender Psychology = The Social Psychology of Gender. - St. Petersburg: Prime Eurosign, 2002.
  11. Maccoby, E. and Jacklin, C. The Psychology of Sex Differences. - Stanford University Press, 1974. - ISBN 9780804708593.
  12. (2009) "Androgen Receptor Repeat Length Polymorphism Associated with Male-to-Female Transsexualism". Biological Psychiatry 65 (1): 93–6. DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.033. PMID 18962445 .
  13. Kruijver F. P., Zhou J. N., Pool C. W., Hofman M. A., Gooren L. J., Swaab D. F. Male-to-female transsexuals have female neuron numbers in a limbic nucleus // The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. - 2000. - No. 85(5). - P. 2034-2041.
  14. (2007) "Male-to-Female Transsexuals Show Sex-Atypical Hypothalamus Activation When Smelling Odorous Steroids". Cerebral Cortex 18 (8): 1900–8. DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhm216. PMID 18056697 .
  15. (2006) "Typical female 2nd–4th finger length (2D:4D) ratios in male-to-female transsexuals-possible implications for prenatal androgen exposure". Psychoneuroendocrinology 31 (2): 265–9. DOI:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.005 . PMID 16140461 .
  16. LeVay S (August 1991). "A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men". Science 253 (5023): 1034–7. DOI:10.1126/science.1887219. PMID 1887219 .
  17. Byne W, Tobet S, Mattiace LA (September 2001). "The interstitial nuclei of the human anterior hypothalamus: an investigation of variation with sex, sexual orientation, and HIV status". Horm Behav 40 (2): 86–92. DOI:10.1006/hbeh.2001.1680. PMID 11534967 .
  18. Eccles, J.S. Bringing young women to math and science // Gender and thought: Psychological perspectives / Crawford, M., and Gentry, M.. - New York: Springer, 1989.
  19. Kimball, M.M. A new perspective on women "s math achievement // Psychological Bulletin. - 1989. - V. 105, No. 2. - S. 198-214.

: the first concept describes social expectations external to a person in connection with his gender, the second - the internal self-perception of a person as a representative of a particular gender. A person's gender identity and gender role may not match - particularly in transgender and intersex people. The alignment of gender role with gender identity is part of the transgender transition.

Gender roles in different cultures

In modern societies, a binary gender system dominates - a way of social organization in which people are divided into two opposite groups - men and women. The binary gender system implies a strict correspondence between sex assigned at birth and gender role, as well as other parameters (in particular, gender identity and sexual orientation). As anthropological studies show, the establishment of such a correspondence is not universal: in many cultures, biological, in particular anatomical sex, does not play a key role in determining gender role or gender identity. Not universal and the allocation of only two genders. For example, many native North American cultures have three or four genders and corresponding gender roles. In West African Yoruba culture, gender is traditionally not a significant social category, and social roles are determined primarily by age and kinship.

Even within close cultures or within the same culture, gender roles can differ markedly. For example, in 18th and 19th century European secular culture, women were expected to be weak and frail, and in most peasant cultures, women were considered to be naturally strong and hardy. In Western (North American and Western European) middle-class cultures since the 1950s, the female gender role has been that of the housewife, and participation in productive work for women has been excluded. Yet at the same time and in the same societies, working outside the home was an expected and self-evident element of the gender role for working-class women. Women's gender role in socialist societies also involved a combination of work outside the home, housework, and family care.

Historical aspect

The ratio of gender roles has changed significantly over the course of human history. In particular, historians note a significant increase in the social role of women in Europe during the late Middle Ages (1300-1500).

Gender Development Explanations

There are two main points of view in the debate about the origin of gender roles and differences: supporters of biological determinism suggest that gender differences are determined by biological, natural factors, and supporters of social constructivism that they are shaped by society in the process of socialization. Various theories of gender development have been put forward in science. Biologically based theories explaining differences in gender roles by evolution have not found convincing empirical evidence. Empirical research has also refuted psychoanalytic theories that explained gender development through the child's relationship with parents. The strongest empirical evidence exists for cognitive and socio-cognitive theories that explain gender development as a complex interplay of biological, cognitive, and social factors.

Viewpoints on the origin of gender roles

Ordinary consciousness often presents the gender roles that exist in a given society in a particular historical period as natural and natural. There are also many studies seeking to uncover the biological basis of gender roles—in particular, to establish the biological origin of gender differences between men and women, and to find the biological causes of gender nonconformity. But the historical and anthropological knowledge accumulated to date does not support this point of view, since the diversity of ideas about gender and gender roles in the cultures of the world and throughout history is too great. At the same time, modern social sciences have collected a lot of data on how gender roles are formed under the influence of various social processes.

Biological determinism

The view that social phenomena are determined by biological factors is called biological determinism. A related concept is naturalization social practices - describes the process of interpreting social practices as facts of nature. Biological determinism in relation to gender roles is expressed, for example, in the widespread assertions that motherhood is a woman's natural destiny, or that men are not naturally emotional.

Since the end of the 19th century, scientists from various scientific fields have conducted many studies of gender differences between men and women. Until the 1970s, the main purpose of these studies was to confirm the biological nature of gender differences and to substantiate the content of existing gender roles. However, the results of most studies show that there are much more similarities between men and women than differences. In a widely cited review study, psychologists Eleanor Maccoby and Carol Jacklin cite four dimensions in which differences between men and women have been found: spatial awareness, mathematical ability, language skills, and aggressiveness. But even these discovered differences are small and strongly depend on the methodology and conditions of the study.

Since the 1970s, scholars have also become interested in the causes of gender non-conformity, i.e. violations of gender roles. Conducted, in particular, research aimed at clarifying the biological causes of transsexuality. There are currently theories linking transsexuality to genetics, brain structure, brain activity, and androgen exposure during fetal development. At the same time, the results of these studies are also controversial - for example, the identified features of the structure of the brain of transsexual people are not unique (similar differences are observed in homosexual people compared to heterosexual people), and there is evidence that the structure of the brain can change under the influence of life experience.

social constructivism

The view that gender roles are formed, or constructed, by society belongs to the theory of social constructivism. The basis for studying the social nature and processes of constructing gender roles was laid, in particular, by the theoretical work of Simone de Beauvoir and Michel Foucault. Studies of the social construction of gender roles show how, in the process of socialization and interaction between people, those gender differences and expectations are formed that are perceived as natural and natural in everyday consciousness.

According to the latest research, the observed differences between men and women are largely due to social factors. For example, research reveals several reasons why women are less successful in mathematics than men: firstly, they lack confidence in their abilities, and secondly, they consider math classes inappropriate for their gender role and refuse them even when show excellent abilities in this area, thirdly, parents and teachers encourage girls to do mathematics much less than boys. Thus, as some researchers note, gender stereotypes work like self-fulfilling prophecies: in the course of socialization, people are given information about gender roles that forms their expectations of themselves, and as a result they show gender-conforming behavior.

Biological theories

Biologically based explanations of gender development and differences are widespread. One of the most influential such theories, evolutionary psychology, explains gender differentiation by heredity. The hereditary origin of gender roles is analyzed through preferences in the choice of sexual partners, reproductive strategies, the contribution of parents to the care of offspring, and the aggressiveness of men. From the point of view of this theory, modern gender roles are due to the successful adaptation of the ancestors of modern man to differences in the reproductive tasks of men and women.

Empirical evidence refutes the main tenets of biological theories of gender development. Many researchers also criticize the methodology of biologically oriented research. Nevertheless, biological theories continue to enjoy great popularity, including among the general public. According to some authors, this is due to the fact that in many societies, ordinary consciousness ascribes to biology the status of absolute truth. In addition, the provisions of biological theories correspond to gender stereotypes.

Reproductive strategies

According to evolutionary psychology, in the process of evolution, different reproductive strategies have been fixed at the genetic level in men and women, dictated by the need to ensure the survival of man as a biological species. The reproductive strategy of men is aimed at maximizing the spread of their genes, so men prefer to have many sexual partners and not spend time caring for offspring. The reproductive strategy of women is to have few sexual partners who will be able to provide themselves and their offspring with the necessary resources for survival.

Many researchers question the very concept of reproductive strategy. From the point of view of the general theory of evolution, natural selection is determined by immediate practical benefits, not by future goals. The claim that ancient men sought to father as many children as possible, and ancient women to find reliable breadwinners, suggests that they had a conscious or unconscious goal, which some authors argue contradicts the Darwinian functional explanation.

Other authors point out that the evolutionary psychology hypothesis is not supported by empirical evidence. In particular, the assumption that ancient women did not have enough food during pregnancy and lactation, looks quite convincing, but with the same success, based on this, it can be assumed that in connection with this, women developed increased abilities for orientation in space and memory , which would allow them to find and remember the location of food sources. Additional information is needed to substantiate any hypothesis about specific adaptive mechanisms. Such information could be data from molecular studies of fossilized human remains or data from archeology, but evolutionary psychology does not offer such data. Some authors note that the concept of reproductive strategies is an attempt to "hindsight" explain modern gender stereotypes.

Anthropological evidence also speaks against the hypothesis of reproductive strategies. They show, in particular, that reproductive behavior is influenced by cultural beliefs about the human body and reproduction. In cultures where multi-partner fertilization is believed to be necessary for reproduction, women have sexual contact with different partners, and these partners are not jealous of each other.

Choice of sexual partners

Evolutionary psychology states that men tend to choose young and physically attractive mates because such mates are more likely to bear healthy offspring, and women tend to choose financially wealthy men who can feed them. In confirmation of these data, the results of surveys are given, in which men and women named the most attractive characteristics of potential partners for themselves. However, numerous studies show that what people say is significantly different from how they actually behave: in fact, physical attractiveness affects the choice of partners in men and women in the same way. On the other hand, indicators of physical attractiveness vary greatly across cultures around the world, and most of these characteristics are not related to fertility. Some authors also point out that evolutionary psychology only explains heterosexual behavior and suggest that evolutionary psychologists avoid looking at research data from non-heterosexual people because their behavior and gender roles do not match gender stereotypes and thus undermine evolutionary explanations.

Aggressive behavior

Psychoanalytic theories

Although psychoanalytic theory has had a major impact on the development of developmental psychology, empirical evidence does not support it. Research has not found a strong relationship between same-sex parent identification and gender role learning. Children's role models are much more likely to be caring or socially powerful adults than to be threatening adults with whom the child has a competitive relationship.

The lack of empirical support for classical psychoanalytic theory has led to the emergence of various updated versions of it. In the field of gender development, one of the most influential recent versions is Nancy Chodorow's theory. According to this theory, gender identity is formed during infancy and not during the phallic phase, as Freud claimed. Both boys and girls initially identify with their mother, but because daughters are the same sex as their mother, identification between daughters and mothers is stronger than between sons and mothers. In the course of further development, girls retain identification with their mother and psychologically merge with her. As a result, the self-image of the girl and the woman is characterized by a strong sense of interdependence, which translates into a desire for interpersonal relationships and encourages the woman, in turn, to become a mother. The development of the boy is determined by the desire to separate from the mother and further define himself through difference from women, which leads to the debasement of femininity.

But the empirical evidence does not support Chodorow's theory either. Research does not find a stronger bond between mothers and daughters than between mothers and sons. There is also no evidence that women's needs for interpersonal relationships are met only through motherhood. On the contrary, studies show that women whose only social role is that of mother and wife are more prone to psychological problems than childless married or unmarried women and working mothers.

Cognitive and social theories

Cognitive and social theories of gender development include theories of cognitive development, gender schemas, social learning, and social cognitive theory. Although at the initial stages these theories differed significantly from each other, and their supporters had heated discussions among themselves, modern versions of these theories have much in common. In general, cognitive and social theories consider gender development as a complex process of interaction of biological, social and cognitive factors. All of them pay significant attention to the social sources of gender development and the active role that a person plays in their own gender development.

Social sources of gender development

The social sources of gender development include, in particular, the influence of parents, other significant adults and peers, as well as information pressure from the media, cinema, literature, etc.

Influence of parents

Differences in the upbringing of boys and girls are described by the concept of "differential socialization". Differential socialization is not necessarily expressed in the form of direct instructions or prohibitions. As studies show, differential socialization begins even before the birth of a child, as soon as its sex is determined using ultrasound. Mothers who learn the sex of their unborn child in this way describe boys as "active" and "mobile", and girls as "calm". From birth, babies tend to be surrounded by gender-specific toys, diapers, and other items; male infants are described as "big", "strong" and "independent", while girls are referred to as "gentle", "delicate" and "beautiful", even if there are no objective differences in the appearance or behavior of the infants. Thus, the ideas and expectations of children related to gender are formed by adults on the basis of gender stereotypes long before the child can begin to show this or that behavior.

Differential socialization continues into the later life of the child. For example, numerous studies show that parents are more stimulating and more responsive to motor activity in infant boys than in girls. Another illustrative experiment concerns the influence of adult gender stereotypes on the choice of toys for children. The experiment was initially carried out with the participation of a three-month-old child, and later again with the participation of several children aged from three to 11 months. Three groups of adults were asked to play with the child, while the first group was told that the child was a girl, the second group that it was a boy, and the third group was not informed about the gender of the child. The adults had three toys at their disposal: a doll, a ball, and a gender-neutral tooth ring. Most of the adults who considered the child to be a boy offered him a ball, and the majority of those who considered the child to be a girl - a doll, without trying to find out which of the toys interests the child more.

Peer Influence

As the child's social world expands, peer groups become another important source of gender development, as well as social learning in general. In interaction with peers, children, starting at the age of three or four, encourage each other for gender-typical behavior, as well as for playing in gender-homogeneous groups, and punish for behavior that is considered inappropriate for their gender.

Information pressure

Finally, the media play a significant role in gender development, especially television, as well as literature, cinema and video games. In these sources from which children receive information about gender roles, men and women are often portrayed in an exaggerated stereotype: men are portrayed as active and adventurous, while women are portrayed as dependent, unambitious and emotional. The depiction of the professional life of men and women often does not correspond to the real state of affairs: men are portrayed as representatives of various professions, leaders and bosses, while women are portrayed as either housewives or working in low-status positions. Such an image does not correspond to the actual statistics of the professional employment of men, nor the wide involvement of women in professional activities. Studies show that gender stereotypes in the media and culture have a big impact on children: those who watch a lot of TV form more stereotyped ideas about gender roles. On the other hand, the non-stereotypical depiction of representatives of different genders expands the range of desires and aspirations in children, as well as the options for roles that they consider acceptable for their gender. The recurring image of equal participation of representatives of different genders in certain activities contributes to the steady softening of gender stereotypes in young children.

Active human role in gender development

Social sources of gender development often provide conflicting information about gender roles and impose conflicting expectations on the child. This requires the child, starting from a very early age, to actively seek and build his own rules and ideas about gender as a new and significant social category for him. Activity in the formation of ideas about gender is manifested, in particular, in selective attention and memory, as well as in the formation of preferences - for example, gender-typical or atypical toys, games with peers of one's own or another gender.

The Importance of Gender Roles

see also

Notes

  1. Nanda, Serena. Gender Diversity: Crosscultural Variations. - Waveland Pr Inc, 1999. - ISBN 978-1577660743.
  2. Roscoe, Will. Changing Ones: Third and Fourth Genders in Native North America. - Palgrave Macmillan, 2000. - ISBN 978-0312224790.
  3. Oyewumi, Oyeronke. Conceptualizing gender: the eurocentric foundations of feminist concepts and the challenge of African epistemologies // Jenda: a Journal of Culture and African Woman Studies. - 2002. - Vol. 2.
  4. Connell R. Gender and Power: Society, Personality, and Gender Politics. - M.: New literary review, 2015. - ISBN 978-5-4448-0248-9.
  5. hooks, bell. Rethinking the Nature of Work // Feminist Theory: From Margin to Center . - Pluto Press, 2000. - ISBN 9780745316635.
  6. Zdravomyslova E., Temkina A. (ed.). Russian gender order: a sociological approach. - St. Petersburg: Publishing House of the European University in St. Petersburg, 2007. - ISBN 978-5-94380-060-3.
  7. , Gender Roles in Late Medieval Europe, p. 297.
  8. Bussey, K., Bandura, A. Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation // Psychological review. - 1999. - T. 106, No. 4. - S. 676-713.
  9. Fausto-Sterling A. Beyond difference: A biologist's perspective // ​​Journal of Social Issues. - 1997. - V. 53, No. 2. - S. 233–258.
  10. Martin, C.L., et al. Cognitive Theories of Early Gender Development // Psychological Bulletin. - 2002. - T. 128, No. 6. - S. 903-933.
  11. Burn Sean. Gender psychology = The Social Psychology of Gender. - St. Petersburg: Prime Eurosign, 2002.
  12. Maccoby, E. and Jacklin, C. The Psychology of Sex Differences. - Stanford University Press, 1974. - ISBN 9780804708593.
  13. Hare, L; Bernard, P; Sanchez, F; Baird, P; Vilain, E; Kennedy, T; Harley, V (2009). “Androgen Receptor Repeat Length Polymorphism Associated with Male-to-Female Transsexualism” . Biological Psychiatry. 65 (1): 93-6. DOI:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.033. PMC. PMID.
  14. Kruijver F. P., Zhou J. N., Pool C. W., Hofman M. A., Gooren L. J., Swaab D. F. Male-to-female transsexuals have female neuron numbers in a limbic nucleus // The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. - 2000. - No. 85(5). - P. 2034-2041.
  15. Berglund, H.; Lindstrom, P.; Dhejne-Helmy, C.; Savic, I. (2007). “Male-to-Female Transsexuals Show Sex-Atypical Hypothalamus Activation When Smelling Odorous Steroids.” Cerebral Cortex. 18 (8): 1900-8. DOI:10.1093/cercor/bhm216. PMID.
  16. Schneider, H; Pickel, J; Stalla, G (2006). “Typical female 2nd–4th finger length (2D:4D) ratios in male-to-female transsexuals-possible implications for prenatal androgen exposure.” Psychoneuroendocrinology. 31 (2): 265-9. DOI:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.07.005 . PMID.
  17. LeVay S (August 1991). “A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men.” Science. 253 (5023): 1034-7. DOI:10.1126/science.1887219. PMID.
  18. Byne W, Tobet S, Mattiace LA; et al. (September 2001). “The interstitial nuclei of the human anterior hypothalamus: an investigation of variation with sex, sexual orientation, and HIV status.” Horm Behav. 40 (2): 86-92. DOI:10.1006/hbeh.2001.1680. PMID.
  19. Eccles, J.S. Bringing young women to math and science // Gender and thought: Psychological perspectives / Crawford, M., and Gentry, M.. - New York: Springer, 1989.
  20. Kimball, M.M. A new perspective on women "s math achievement // Psychological Bulletin. - 1989. - T. 105, No. 2. - S. 198-214.
  21. Dweck, C. S. et al. Sex differences in learned helplessness: II. The contingencies of evaluative feedback in the classroom and III. An experimental analysis // Developmental psychology. - 1978. - V. 14, No. 3. - S. 268-276.